Free Essay

Size Task

In: Social Issues

Submitted By SarahLouis
Words 8356
Pages 34
Psychophysiology, 43 (2006), 207–215. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2006 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00392.x

When does size not matter? Effects of stimulus size on affective modulation

ANDREA DE CESAREI AND MAURIZIO CODISPOTI
Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy

Abstract Motivationally relevant stimuli have been shown to receive prioritized processing compared to neutral stimuli at distinct processing stages. This effect has been related to the evolutionary importance of rapidly detecting dangers and potential rewards and has been shown to be modulated by the distance between an organism and a faced stimulus. Similarly, recent studies showed degrees of emotional modulation of autonomic responses and subjective arousal ratings depending on stimulus size. In the present study, affective modulation of pictures presented in different sizes was investigated by measuring event-related potentials during a two-choice categorization task. Results showed significant emotional modulation across all sizes at both earlier and later stages of processing. Moreover, affective modulation of earlier processes was reduced in smaller compared to larger sizes, whereas no changes in affective modulation were observed at later stages. Descriptors: Emotion, Attention, Categorization, Stimulus size, ERPs

Although objects may appear under very different viewing conditions (e.g., visual angle, viewing time, orientation, brightness), the human visual system has a remarkable capacity to recognize them and to organize its response adaptively. However, behavior is organized both on the basis of stimulus significance and of contextual cues, which further define the relevance of the stimulus for the observer. In the present study we investigate the hypothesis that the emotional response to biologically relevant stimuli (Bradley, 2000; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1997) may vary according to stimulus size. Retinal size is determined both by the physical size of the stimulus and on the distance from the observer, and could play a role in modulating emotional response. In particular, assuming the view that emotions evolved from simpler reactions to biologically relevant threatening or appetitive stimuli, responses to stimuli that are bigger or nearer (e.g., food or an attacking animal) should be particularly pronounced. Both animal studies and human research on phobic and normal individuals described a gradient of behavioral and physiological responses depending on the distance between the organism and the encountered stimulus (Blanchard & Blanchard, 2003; Fanselow, 1994; Lang et al., 1997; Teghtsoonian & Frost, 1982). These patterns of emotional response have been related to the different engagement of the motivational systems depending on the distance from the faced stimulus. Similarly, Miller (1959) described distinct gradients of approach and
The authors thank Simone Biondi for his help in data collection. We also thank all the people who volunteered to participate in the study. Address reprint requests to: Maurizio Codispoti, Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Viale Berti Pichat, 5F40127 Bologna, Italy. E-mail: maurizio.codispoti@unibo.it. 207

avoidance when the organism is facing a stimulus that has positive or negative significance, reflecting different behavior according to the distance and the valence of the stimulus. Consistent with the prediction of larger emotional response to nearer stimuli, a study comparing the reaction of snake-phobic participants to snakes presented at various distances (Teghtsoonian & Frost, 1982) showed a linear increase of autonomic responses and self-reported fear as a function of distance. Because distance and retinal size are strictly related (Loftus & Harley, 2005), it can be expected that changes in stimulus size determine arousal modulations similarly to distance. Moreover, in an evolutionary framework, the physical size of an encountered object or organism may determine the motivational relevance for the observer. This possibility is supported by the results of a recent study (Reeves, Lang, Kim, & Tatar, 1999) that investigated autonomic response following arousing and nonarousing stimuli presented in different sizes, suggesting a more pronounced emotional response for bigger compared to smaller stimuli. In the present study, we assessed the possibility that changes in stimulus size may influence the affective modulation of early and late event-related potentials (ERPs). Recently, a number of studies have demonstrated that ERPs may provide useful information regarding early processing of natural emotional stimuli (Junghofer, Bradley, Elbert, & Lang, ¨ 2001; Muller, Keil, Gruber, & Elbert, 1999; Schupp, Junghofer, ¨ ¨ Weike, & Hamm, 2003, 2004). In particular, a modulation on the basis of stimulus arousal has been observed beginning at around 150 ms poststimulus and lasting approximately 200 ms. Arousing pictures elicit a less pronounced positivity over temporo-occipital areas that is accompanied by a polarity inversion at frontal leads. Although it has been suggested that arousing pictures receive

208 prioritized processing because of their intrinsic relevance (Schupp et al., 2003), neutral stimuli can also result in a similar pattern of activation if they are explicitly made relevant to the task (Codispoti, Ferrari, Junghofer, & Schupp, 2006; Thorpe, ¨ Fize, & Marlot, 1996). In target detection paradigms, where one class of stimuli is explicitly made task relevant on the basis of features such as color, spatial frequency, or shape, a less positive ERP is observed over occipital areas (Harter & Aine, 1984; Hillyard & Anllo-Vento, 1998; Mangun, 1995; Ritter, Simson, & Vaughan, 1983). It has been suggested that these effects reflect an early attentional selection process on the basis of stimulus relevance (Hillyard & Anllo-Vento, 1998). Source analysis procedures and brain imaging techniques suggest that generators of this activity are situated in the extrastriate visual cortex. This area is connected via feedforward and feedback connections, both with lower-order visual areas and with other brain structures as the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala. Higher-order structures would exert a top-down control on early visual processing (Bar, 2003; Freedman, Riesenhuber, Poggio, & Miller, 2003; Ghashghaei & Barbas, 2002; Rolls, 2000; Sigala & Logotethis, 2002), prompting the visual system to efficiently respond to relevant stimuli. While early perceptual processes are specific to stimulus modality and rely more on perceptual features, subsequent processing operates on the semantic outcome of categorization processes and should be less influenced by perceptual features. The late positive potential (Kok, 1997, 2001; Ruchkin & Sutton 1983) is a slow positive deflection of the ERP that is observed at centroparietal sensor sites and reflects stages of processing that follow stimulus identification and are modulated by task requirements (Kok, 1997, 2001). Interestingly, when motivationally relevant scenes are presented, a larger late positive potential is observed compared to neutral stimuli (Cacioppo, Crites, Berntson, & Coles, 1993; Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer, & Lang, 2000; Schupp et al., 2000). This effect is equally pronounced for pleasant and unpleasant stimuli and has been linked to the concept of motivated attention, which suggests that detection of relevant stimuli may engage attentional resources and activate selective attention processes that further elaborate stimulus contents (Bradley, 2000; Lang et al., 1997). Moreover, affective modulation of the late positive potential covaries with both subjective arousal ratings and autonomic responses (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Lang et al., 1997), reflecting the role of late evaluative processes in regulating patterns of autonomic and behavioral responses. To test the possibility that emotional response may vary according to stimulus size, images differing in emotional content and size were presented to participants, who were asked to categorize them as representing animals or people. Whereas earlier ERP components (e.g., the P1/N1) were expected to vary as a function of perceptual size (Busch, Debener, Kranczioch, Engel, & Herrmann, 2004), ERPs during the 150–300 ms and 400–600 ms time windows were used to assess emotional modulation of picture processing (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Junghofer et al., 2001). ¨ Assuming that size reduction determines lower relevance of the scene to the observer, a reduction in affective modulation at both early and late stages of processing was expected for smaller compared to larger images. Alternatively, if stimulus size does not modulate the degree of activation induced by the stimulus, no change in affective modulation at either the early or late time interval was expected. Finally, because size reduction also results in decreased discriminability due to the loss of fine details in the

A. De Cesarei and M. Codispoti scene (Loftus & Harley, 2005), we expected that the earlier time window, reflecting emotional modulation of stages of perceptual analysis, would have been more affected by size reduction compared to the late positive potential, which is thought to reflect processes initiated after stimulus recognition.

Methods Participants Sixteen students (8 women) from the University of Bologna volunteered to participate in the present study and signed an informed consent form. Age ranged from 20 to 29 (average 5 22.7, SD 5 2.27). All participants were right-handed. They were recruited through advertisements and agreed to participate in the present study after having been fully informed about the experimental material and the duration. Materials and Design Two hundred sixty-four color pictures were selected from various sources, including the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2001), the Internet, and document scanning. Each picture belonged to one of four categories: animals (n 5 132), erotic couples (n 5 44), neutral people (n 5 44), and mutilated people (n 5 44). Pictures in full size covered the whole screen area and subtended a visual angle of 21.231 (horizontal) Â 16.221 (vertical). Three additional size conditions were created by resizing each picture to 50%, 25%, and 12.5% of the original size, corresponding respectively to horizontal and vertical visual angles of 10.711 Â 8.21 (50%), 5.361 Â 4.11 (25%), and 2.681 Â 2.051 (12.5%). The original height/width ratio was preserved after resizing. In the remainder, each size condition will be labeled as the percentage of original size (100%, 50%, 25%, 12.5%). Stimuli were presented on a 19-in. CRT monitor, at 800 Â 600 resolution and a refresh rate of 155 Hz, controlled by an IBM computer and E-Prime software (Schneider, Eschman, & Zuccolotto, 2002). Images were edited to adjust brightness (defined as average RGB energy) and contrast to the same overall value (respectively 155 and 25 on a scale ranging from 0 to 255). Then, images were pasted at the center of a gray background. Because progressively larger portions of gray background are projected on the retina with decreasing picture size, brightness differences between the background and the picture (e.g., background darker than the picture) could have led to overall brightness changes across sizes. To avoid this potential confounding factor, background brightness was equated to the average brightness of all pictures. The experimental session was divided into eight blocks, to enhance the signal-to-noise ratio of ERPs by increasing the number of trials. Each block consisted of the presentation of the entire 264-image data set. Across blocks, the 264 images were presented in a different order. Within each block, 66 pictures were presented in each size. To prevent participants from recognizing a small picture just because the same image was presented in a bigger size in a previous block, we let each participant see each picture in only one size. Therefore, each image was repeated eight times to a single participant, each time in the same size. Procedure Participants sat in front of the computer monitor, with their head supported by a chin rest. The distance between the eyes and the

Stimulus size and affective modulation monitor was 100 cm. Each block was started by the experimenter, and lasted about 15 min. During each trial, a fixation cross was presented on a gray background for 500 ms. Then the image was displayed for 100 ms, followed by a gray screen which lasted 1000 ms. During this time, participants had to respond as to whether the image represented an animal or a person by pressing one of two mouse buttons with the index finger of the dominant hand. Finally, after a variable delay between 500 and 1500 ms, the next trial began. EEG Recording and Processing EEG was recorded at a sampling rate of 500 Hz and a resolution of 0.12 mV from 59 active sites referenced to Cz using an SA Instrumentation Co. (San Diego, CA) UF-64/72BA amplifier and in-house developed acquisition software. Impedance of each sensor was kept below 10 kO. EEG was on-line filtered from 0.01 to 100 Hz. Eye movements were recorded from two bipolar couples of electrodes, placed, respectively, 1 cm above and below the right eye and 1 cm left and right to the side of the eyes. Ocular signal was recorded with a resolution of 0.24 mVat a sample rate of 500 Hz and was on-line filtered from 0.01 to 100 Hz. Off-line analysis was performed using Emegs (Junghofer & Peyk, 2004) ¨ and included removal of eye movements from the EEG signal (Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1983), low-pass filtering at 30 Hz, artifact detection, sensor interpolation (Junghofer, Elbert, Tuck¨ er, & Rockstroh, 2000) and re-referencing to the average of all channels. Finally, a baseline correction based on the 100 ms prior to stimulus onset was performed. Processed data were averaged according to the factors Category and Size. Only trials where participants correctly categorized stimuli were analyzed. Scoring of ERP Components Averaged waveforms were calculated for each participant and experimental condition. ERPs in the 150–300 ms time window were scored as average at occipito-temporal sensor sites (TP7, TP8, P7/T5, P5, P6, P8/ T6, PO7, PO5, PO3, POz, PO4, PO6, PO8) in the time interval between 150 and 300 ms after stimulus onset. Additional analyses were aimed at detailing amplitude and latency of early affective modulation. Therefore, when a significant difference between either pleasant–neutral or unpleasant–neutral was observed, the corresponding difference waveform was calculated and the negative peak1 between 150 and 300 ms was scored in amplitude and latency. Finally, the onset of early affective modulation was also assessed. With this aim, when a significant effect involving picture content was observed, corresponding waveforms were compared by performing consecutive t tests at each time point on the average of the occipito-temporal sensor group. A sequence of 30 consecutive significant (a 5 .01) t tests, corresponding to a time window of 60 ms, was used as a threshold at which a modulation would be accepted as significant. This approach has been recommended by recent ERP guidelines (Luck, 2005), and allows the determination of the onset of a significant
1 Peaks of P1, N1, and of the differential waveforms in the 150–300 ms time interval were determined by identifying, in each nonflat time interval of interest, the 10 points where the absolute value of the gradient (distance between each point and the following) is minimum. This procedure allows the identification of ERP peaks within the selected interval. Among these values, the most positive (or negative) value was interpreted as the peak of the component of interest and scored in amplitude and latency. This procedure achieves good results in determining peaks, and has the advantage of providing accurate amplitude and latency scores while avoiding problems related to latency shifts and local spurious maxima.

209 difference between experimental conditions. Moreover, it has been successfully employed in recent investigations related to the present study (Rugg, Doyle, & Wells, 1995; Thorpe et al., 1996; VanRullen & Thorpe, 2001). The late positive potential was scored as the average ERP amplitude between 400 and 600 ms at centro-parietal sensor sites (FC3, FC1, FCz, FC2, FC4, C3, C1, Cz, C2, C4, CP3, CP1, CPz, CP2, CP4, P3, P1, Pz, P2, P4, PO3, POz, PO4). Additionally, the P1 and the N1 components were scored in amplitude and latency, respectively, on the peak of the positive and negative component in the time windows between 60 and 100 ms and 90 and 150 ms at occipital sensor sites (O1, Oz, O2). Behavioral Variables Response time and accuracy were collected using E-Prime (Schneider et al., 2002). Analysis of response times was performed only on correct trials. Data Analysis The focus of the present study was to investigate the effects of stimulus size on affective modulation. Animal pictures were added to have an explicit categorization task that could allow the examination of the capability to recognize, in all sizes, the content of the presented stimulus. For clarity, we decided to discard animals from the main analysis reported and to focus on human content stimuli.2 All data (ERPs and behavioral responses) were analyzed with a repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with factors Category (3 levels, pleasant, neutral, unpleasant) and Size (4 levels).3,4When a significant main effect or interaction was found, additional tests were carried out to describe the direction of the effects. In particular, pairwise comparisons (pleasant vs. neutral, unpleasant vs. neutral, and pleasant vs. unpleasant) were used to better delineate Category effects, and linear trend analysis was carried out when examining the effects of Size. All ANOVAs were conducted using a multivariate test statistic (Wilks’ lambda) as
2 A preliminary ANOVA with factors Size (4 levels) and Category (2 levels, animals vs. neutral) compared ERPs to animal to neutral pictures. Although more positivity in the 150–300 ms time window was observed for animals compared to neutral stimuli, F(1,15) 5 13.6, po.01, Z2 5 .48, and earlier peaking of the N1 was found for animals compared to neutral stimuli, F(1,15) 5 6.69, po.05, Z2 5 .31, in none of the examined time intervals was a significant interaction of Size and Category observed. No other significant effects were observed in this analysis. Therefore, picture size similarly affected animal and neutral picture processing in early and late time intervals. No other significant effects or interactions with size were observed. 3 A preliminary analysis involving the factors of Category, Size, and Laterality was performed on the amplitudes and latencies of all considered ERP components, on the same regions of interest reported in the Methods section but excluding midline sensors. Peak amplitude of the N1 yielded a significant Laterality  Size interaction, F(3,13) 5 7.04, po.01, Z2 5 .62, showing more pronounced N1 amplitude reduction over left (linear contrast F[1,15] 5 16.26, po.001, Z2 5 .52) compared to right regions (linear contrast F[1,15] 5 9.83, po.05, Z2 5 .40). The late positive potential was more prominent on the right, factor Laterality F(1,15) 5 23.4, po.001, Z2 5 .61. In addition, an almost significant Laterality  Size interaction, F(3,13) 5 3.4, p 5 .05, Z2 5 .44, indicates more pronounced effects of size over left (linear contrast F[1,15] 5 66.13, po.001, Z2 5 .82) compared to the left areas (linear contrast F[1,15] 5 55.53, po.001, Z2 5 .79). No other significant effects or interactions involving factor Laterality were observed. 4 To control for repetition effects, an ANOVA with the factors of Block (8 levels), Category (4 levels), and Size (4 levels) was performed. No significant effects involving the Block factor were found in this analysis.

210

A. De Cesarei and M. Codispoti

Figure 1. Global field power and averaged waveforms from PO8, P2, and O2, in all size conditions.

suggested by Vasey and Thayer (1987), to avoid the inflated type I error rate associated with the univariate ANOVA when the sphericity assumption is not met. For each ANOVA test, we calculated and report the partial eta squared, which reflects the proportion of variance that is accounted for by experimental manipulations. Results Figure 1a illustrates the global field power in all size and category conditions, providing information that is independent of the choice of regions of interest. From left to right, each plot represents a size condition ranging from smallest (12.5%, left end) to largest (100%, right end). Progressively larger amplitude of the global field power was observed as the image was made larger. Moreover, category effects (color lines) were observed at both earlier and later latencies. Focusing on one representative electrode from each region of interest, Figure 1b–d illustrates the combined effects of size and category on specific ERP components. In the 150–300 ms time interval, pleasant pictures appeared to be differentiated from neutral and unpleasant stimuli. Furthermore, this difference appeared more pronounced in larger compared to smaller sizes. Replicating previous findings, the late positive potential was larger for arousing (pleasant and unpleasant) compared to neutral pictures. Interestingly, affective modulation of the late positive potential appeared mainly unaffected by changes in image size. In the following, we report statistical analyses regarding ERP effects in all examined time intervals and behavioral responses. Early Modulation (150–300 ms) Figure 2 illustrates scalp topographies of the early emotional modulation. A broad positivity, more pronounced for neutral and unpleasant compared to pleasant stimuli, is observed over occipito-temporal regions. Analysis of ERP amplitude in the

early time window yielded a significant Category effect, F(2,14) 5 46.68, po.001, Z2 5 .87. However, whereas significant differences between pleasant stimuli versus all other categories were observed, pso.001, no difference between unpleasant and neutral stimuli was found, p>.05. A significant effect of Size was also observed in this time interval, F(3,13) 5 13.73, po.001, Z2 5 .75, characterized by a significant linear trend F(1,15) 5 36.33, po.001, Z2 5 .71, indicating progressively less pronounced positivity for smaller compared to larger stimuli. Furthermore, as illustrated in Figure 2, a significant Size  Category interaction pointed out that affective modulation of early ERP amplitude varied with picture size, F(6,10) 5 5.08, po.05, Z2 5 .75. Following this significant interaction, affective modulation of ERP amplitude in the early time interval was analyzed at each picture size. A significant Category effect was observed in all size conditions, Fs(2,14)>15.98, pso.001, Z2>.70 indicating that, in each size, early ERP amplitude to pleasant stimuli was differentiated from neutral and unpleasant pictures, pso.001, and no affective modulation was observed for unpleasant compared to neutral stimuli. Because only the pleasant–neutral difference reached significance, we decided to focus further analyses on the early affective modulation for pleasant compared to neutral scenes. To detail size effects on early affective modulation, the difference between ERPs elicited by pleasant and neutral stimuli was calculated in each size. Figure 3 illustrates differential waveforms over occipito-temporal sensors (upper panel) and topography of the time course of early affective ERP modulation (lower panel). When stimuli were presented in full size, a negative difference was observed at occipito-temporal leads between about 130 and 300 ms. This effect appeared both reduced in amplitude and delayed with decreasing picture size. A significant effect of Size was observed on the amplitude of the peak of the pleasant–neutral difference wave, F(3,13) 5 9.61, po.001,

Stimulus size and affective modulation

211 tral difference was also significant, F(3,13) 5 26.84, po.001, Z2 5 .86, linear trend F(1,15) 5 76.90, po.001, Z2 5 .84, showing that the peak of the pleasant–neutral differential was delayed in smaller compared to larger sizes. Finally, the onset of the early affective modulation was also assessed. With this aim, waveforms were compared via consecutive t tests (see Methods). Results showed that the onset of the differentiation between pleasant and neutral stimuli was shifted in latency as a function of size, starting at 136 ms in the 100% size condition, 152 ms in 50%, 166 ms in 25%, and 196 ms in the 12.5% condition (see Figure 3, upper panel). Late Positive Potential Topographies of the late positive potential are shown in Figure 4. A positive ERP was evident over parietal sensors, which was more positively pronounced for pleasant and unpleasant compared to neutral stimuli. Accordingly, analysis of the amplitude of the late positive potential yielded a significant effect of Category, F(2,14) 5 11.05, po0.001, Z2 5 0.61, indicating more positivity for both pleasant and unpleasant compared to neutral stimuli, pso.001. Additionally, pleasant stimuli elicited more positivity compared to unpleasant pictures, po.05. Stimulus size significantly modulated amplitude of the late positive potential, F(3,13) 5 15.57, po0.001, Z2 5 0.78, linear trend F(1,15) 5 43.13, po.001, Z2 5 .74, eliciting a less pronounced late positive potential for smaller compared to larger stimuli. The interaction Category  Size did not reach significance, F(6,10) 5

Figure 2. Scalp maps of activity over posterior areas, in the 150–300 ms time interval, for all sizes and categories.

Z2 5 .69, linear trend F(1,15) 5 29.43, po.001, Z2 5 .66, indicating less negative differential for smaller compared to larger stimuli. Analysis of the latency of the peak of the pleasant–neu-

Figure 3. Size effects on early pleasant–neutral modulation. The same time scale applies to both waveforms and scalp maps. a: Differential waveforms over occipito-temporal sensor group, in all sizes. Horizontal bars highlight time intervals where a statistically significant differentiation was observed. b: Topographies of the difference of pleasant-neutral over posterior areas, in all sizes.

212

A. De Cesarei and M. Codispoti ited a more delayed P1 compared to larger pictures, F(3,13) 5 7.04, po.01, Z2 5 .62, linear trend F(1,15) 5 15.92, po.001, Z2 5 .52. Finally, no interaction between the factors Category and Size was observed on P1 latency. With regard to the N1 component, no main Category effect was observed on peak amplitude. A significant main effect of Size indicated that, with decreasing stimulus size, amplitude of the N1 was reduced, F(3,13) 5 7.6, po.01, Z2 5 .64, linear trend F(1,15) 5 20.96, po.001, Z2 5 .58. No interaction between the factors Category and Size was observed on the amplitude of the N1 component. With regards to N1 latency, no main effect of Category was observed. Instead, a significant effect of the Size factor indicated generally longer N1 peak latencies following presentation of smaller compared to larger stimuli, F(3,13) 5 16.77, po.001, Z2 5 .80, linear trend F(1,15) 5 19.3, po.001, Z2 5 .56. Finally, a significant Category  Size interaction was observed, F(6,10) 5 4.48, po.05, Z2 5 .73, showing earlier peaking of the N1 for pleasant pictures compared to other categories in the 100% size condition, pso.05, and no difference between categories in other sizes.

Figure 4. Scalp maps of activity over centro-parietal areas, in the 400– 600 ms time interval, for all sizes and categories.

2.75, n.s. Consistently, Category effects were significant in all sizes, Fs(2,14)>7.4, pso.01, Z2>.51, indicating more positivity for pleasant and unpleasant compared to neutral stimuli, pso.05. To visually compare size effects on early and late ERP components, Figure 5 reports size effects on affective modulation at both stages. Whereas affective modulation of the early component showed a marked decrease with picture size, emotional effects in the 400–600 ms window appeared stable across all sizes. Additional Analyses: P1/N1 No main effect or interaction of Category and Size was observed on the amplitude of the P1 component. With regards to the latency of the P1 peak, a significant Category effect was observed, F(2,14) 5 9.66, po.01, Z2 5 .58, indicating delayed peak latency for unpleasant compared to pleasant and neutral pictures, pso.05. A main effect of Size indicated that smaller stimuli elic-

Behavioral Responses Mean and standard deviations of response times and accuracy per condition are shown in Table 1. Analysis of response times yielded a significant Category effect, F(2,14) 5 22.88, po.001, Z2 5 .77, indicating faster responses to pleasant stimuli compared to neutral and unpleasant pictures, and slower responses to unpleasant stimuli compared to pleasant and neutrals, all pso.01. A significant effect of Size was also observed, slowing response times as stimuli were made smaller, F(3,13) 5 17.74, po.001, Z2 5 .8, linear trend F(1,15) 5 59.45, po.001, Z2 5 .8. Finally, a significant interaction between Category and Size was observed, F(6,10) 5 3.40, po.05, Z2 5 .67, indicating that the slowing of responses across sizes was more pronounced for negative compared to neutral and pleasant pictures, contrast unpleasant versus neutral and positive across sizes F(1,15) 5 23.78, po.001, Z2 5 .61. Analysis of Accuracy yielded a significant Category effect F(2,14) 5 20.74, po.001, Z2 5 .75, indicating a more accurate performance for pleasant compared to neutral and unpleasant stimuli and more errors for unpleasant stimuli compared to pleasant and neutrals pictures, pso.01. Following size reduction, a significant decrease in accuracy was observed, F(3,13) 5 13.19, po.001, Z2 5 .75, linear trend F(1,15) 5 32.4, po.001, Z2 5 .68. This effect was further characterized by an interaction with Category, F(6,10) 5 6.16, po.01, Z2 5 .79, indicating a more pronounced decrease for unpleasant pictures across sizes, contrast unpleasant versus neutral and positive across sizes F(1,15) 5 29.21, po.001, Z2 5 .66. Discussion In a highly complex environment where potentially threatening and appetitive items are distributed, emotional categorization serves the adaptive function of identifying an object or individual as a predator or a potential reward, and consequently organizing responses. In the present study we assessed the possibility that, similar to distance, stimulus size may also modulate affective processing. Consistent with previous studies, emotional content modulated both early and late ERP components. In particular, pleasant pictures elicited a less pronounced positivity in the 150–300 ms time interval and both pleasant and unpleasant pic-

Figure 5. Scatterplot and linear fit of affective modulation in the four sizes, separately for early and late ERP intervals.

Stimulus size and affective modulation
Table 1. Means (Standard Deviation) of Response Times (in Milliseconds) and Accuracy (Percentage Correct) for Each Affective Content and Stimulus Size
Pleasant RTs 100% 50% 25% 12.5% Total 572.65 (68.80) 585.61 (64.28) 596.05 (75.02) 622.51 (78.62) 594.2 (72.55) Accuracy 99 (1.16) 97.8 (1.73) 98.72 (1.37) 97.09 (3.32) 98.15 (2.17) RTs 612.16 (86.94) 620.92 (95.24) 637.4 (113.42) 656.16 (104.30) 631.66 (99.49) Neutral Accuracy 97.51 (2.73) 96.87 (3.31) 96.38 (3.14) 94.1 (5.40) 96.22 (3.92) RTs 613.63 (77.94) 630.42 (96.56) 653.02 (83.68) 701.14 (110.19) 649.55 (96.56) Unpleasant Accuracy 97.73 (1.95) 94.03 (4.95) 93.61 (3.96) 84.23 (9.06) 92.4 (7.43) RTs 599.48 (78.93) 612.32 (87.00) 628.82 (93.42) 659.94 (101.88) 625.14 (92.80) Total

213

Accuracy 98.08 (2.11) 96.23 (3.86) 96.24 (3.63) 91.81 (8.36) 95.59 (5.53)

tures were differentiated from neutral pictures in the 400–600 ms time interval. When stimuli were presented in small size, a reduction and delay in emotional modulation was observed only on the early ERP time interval examined. Although previous studies showed lower arousal ratings for stimuli presented in smaller sizes, in the present study affective modulation of the late positive potential was not decreased following size reduction. It has been suggested that a perceptual processing stage that can be modulated by implicit and explicit stimulus relevance can be reflected in early (150–300 ms) ERP components. Current models on early visual processing suggest that information coming from the retina into primary visual areas V1 and V2 is roughly analyzed and projected to other brain structures such as the prefrontal lobe and the amygdala, which in turn send reentrant feedback that can modulate subsequent activity of the primary and secondary visual areas (Bar, 2003; Bradley et al., 2003; Buchel & Friston, 1997; Rolls, 2000; Stefanacci & Amaral, ¨ 2002). However, there are several limitations, for instance, regarding color or fine detail information, in the quality of visual information that can be conveyed at this time. Consequently, this biasing mechanism is particularly likely to be disrupted by a change in discriminability of the scene. Accordingly, when discriminability of the image is reduced, early modulation in the 150– 300 ms latency range is largely reduced, possibly because structures producing top-down control do not receive enough information to bias early visual processing (Goffaux, Gauthier, & ´ Rossion, 2003; Mace, Thorpe, & Fabre-Thorpe, 2005). Similarly, image complexity has been shown to modulate ERPs in the same latency range (Bradley, 2002; Low, Lang, & Bradley, 2005). ¨ On the other hand, in the present study no decrease in affective modulation of the late positive potential was observed following size reduction. The late positive potential represents the ERP correlate of postperceptual processes linked to stimulus evaluation, and is modulated by attentional and motivational factors (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Keil et al., 2002; Kok, 2001; Ruchkin & Sutton, 1983). Whereas early perceptual processes are aimed at understanding the content of a scene and analyzing incoming information from sensory organs (e.g., the retina), later evaluative processes operate on more semantic representations. At this stage, deployment of attentional resources to motivationally relevant items is not expected to be modulated by perceptual characteristics of the stimulus (cf. Ito & Cacioppo, 2004). However, a less pronounced late positive potential was observed for smaller compared to larger images. This result could relate to either lower arousal or discriminability of smaller images. Previous studies showed lower subjective arousal ratings and less pronounced autonomic responses to pictures presented in smaller compared to larger sizes (Detenber & Reeves, 1996; Reeves et al.,

1999). Because amplitude of the late positive potential covaries both with self-reported arousal and skin conductance level (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Lang et al., 1997), present results could reflect lower arousal of smaller pictures. Alternatively, amplitude change of the late positive potential could relate to lower discriminability of smaller images compared to larger ones. However, a larger late positive potential has been observed following presentation of degraded visual stimuli (Kok, 2001; Kok & DeJong, 1981). Therefore, it seems unlikely that present results reflect changes in stimulus discriminability. As Norman and Bobrow (1975) noted, distinct cognitive processes are sensitive to different manipulations, with some being limited by the quality of perceptual data and others by the amount of allowable resources. In this respect, although similarities between affective modulation occurring during early and late time windows have been repeatedly observed, size manipulation allowed the disentanglement of a first stage of processing that can be modulated on the basis of extrinsic and intrinsic relevance, but that relies on the quality of perceptual input, and a later stage that is sensitive to relevance manipulations but has been shown to be less sensitive to perceptual manipulations. Size reduction is also associated with changes in a number of dimensions. Features such as the distribution of spatial frequencies and number of details vary as pictures are made smaller, leading to an overall greater difficulty in discriminating the content of small pictures (Loftus & Harley, 2005). In the present study, this trend was corroborated by a linear, significant impairment of accuracy and response times for smaller images, which was reflected on both P1 and N1. Similarly, perceptual factors may also explicate the lack of early differentiation between unpleasant and neutral stimuli. In the present study, unpleasant pictures were shown to be the most difficult images to discriminate, and this class of stimuli was more affected by size reduction. In conditions where the time allowed to process a scene is short, more difficult images may take more time to be fully processed. Consistently, at later time intervals (400–600 ms), unpleasant pictures were significantly differentiated from neutral pictures. Consistent with this result, in a recent study (Schupp et al., 2004), less pronounced emotional effects in the 150–300 ms time interval for unpleasant than pleasant pictures were observed. Therefore, although suggesting that discrimination difficulty may be unequally distributed among categories, we remark the need expressed by Schupp and colleagues (2004) for more studies investigating emotional response to stimuli belonging to different categories under different perceptual and attentional conditions. Event-related cortical activity is observed following timelocked events, including image onset and offset. In the present

214 study, as pictures were presented for a short presentation time and no transition separated pictures from the subsequent blank screen, the abrupt offset could have elicited a potential that overlapped the picture ERP. However, because all images were presented under the same conditions, the offset potential should not differ from category to category. Moreover, this choice is also associated with two main advantages. First, because the picture vanished very early, participants were unlikely to execute exploratory eye movements to scan pictures presented in maximal size. If this had been the case, eye movements would have differentially contaminated conditions where stimuli were presented in large compared to small size, constituting a potential confounding factor. The second advantage is that, because presentation time was below the time usually indicated as necessary to perform a saccade (about 150 ms), present data could be extended by future studies presenting natural complex pictures in the parafovea (cf. Calvo & Lang, 2005; Keil, Moratti, Sabatinelli, Bradley, & Lang, 2004; Perlman et al., 2005). In the present study, affective modulation was investigated using one specific semantic content for each picture category (pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant), and affective modulation was evaluated by means of cortical responses. Future studies might further examine the effect of picture size on affective modulation using a broader spectrum (heart rate, skin conductance response, startle reflex, corrugator activity) of psychophysiological responses as well as other semantic categories for each valence (e.g., human and animal attack, babies, food, contamination; Bradley, Codispoti, Cuthbert, & Lang, 2001). Moreover, in the present study, pictures represented people either in full view or half view or otherwise only showed body details. Consistently, pictures of close-up subjects (e.g., faces) were displayed in a size that was close to real size, whereas pictures of context (e.g., people in urban or natural environments) were displayed in a size that was smaller than the real size of the person. Although in the present study all pictures were presented in all sizes, therefore preventing effects related to an unbalanced scaling factor, it would be interesting to investigate the role of the scaling factor in modulating affective processing. Based on present results, a straightforward prediction would be that, like image size, the scaling factor could modulate early but not late affective processing.

A. De Cesarei and M. Codispoti A final remark concerns the use of pictures of different sizes when investigating emotional modulation. Across different experiments, there is a high variance in the size of pictures, ranging from 5.71 Â 4.61 to 251 Â 18.751 horizontal and vertical degrees of visual angle (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Keil et al., 2004; Kimura, Yoshino, Takahashi, & Nomura, 2004; Pessoa, McKenna, Gutierrez, & Ungerleider, 2002). In spite of the possibility that stimulus size may modulate emotional responses, a systematic investigation of the effects of size on emotional modulation has not been conducted yet. In this respect, in the present study distinct ERP modulations were differently sensitive to size reduction, suggesting that the presentation of stimuli in small sizes could affect specific processes, potentially obscuring emotional effects occurring with larger stimuli. However, to validate present observations with regards to a wider set of stimuli and conditions, more studies varying semantic category and viewing conditions are needed. Moreover, present results were obtained with centrally presented images whose projection lies on the fovea, therefore benefiting from maximal visual acuity. When stimuli are presented in the periphery of the visual field, a further decrease in discriminability occurs, therefore potentially impairing categorization processes (cf. Rousselet, 2005).

Summary Although reductions in emotional modulation were observed at specific processing stages, all emotional effects observed for the largest size were significant even in the 2.681 Â 2.051 presentation condition. This finding is particularly noteworthy and provides evidence of the efficiency of the human visual system in detecting relevant stimuli and allocating resources to their processing, even when they are presented in a degraded form. Interestingly, different affective reactions seem to reflect different processes. Although autonomic responses to emotional pictures, probably reflecting processes related to sensory intake and preparation for action (Codispoti, Ferrari, & Bradley, 2006), are affected by stimulus size (Reeves et al., 1999), stimulus categorization and affective modulation reflected in ERPs seem to suggest that modulation of early processes is more affected by stimulus size compared to later stages of emotional perception.

REFERENCES
Bar, M. (2003). A cortical mechanism for triggering top-down facilitation in visual object recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 600–609. Blanchard, R. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (2003). Bringing natural behaviors into the laboratory: A tribute to Paul MacLean. Physiology and Behavior, 79, 515–524. Bradley, M. M. (2000). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 602–642). New York: Cambridge University Press. Bradley, M. M. (2002). Natural selective attention: Emotion in perception. Presidential Address at the 42nd Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychophysiological Research, Washington, D.C., October. Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298. Bradley, M. M., Sabatinelli, D., Lang, P. J., Fitzsimmons, J. R., King, W., & Desai, P. (2003). Activation of the visual cortex in motivated attention. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 369–380. Buchel, C., & Friston, K. J. (1997). Modulation of connectivity in ¨ visual pathways by attention: Cortical interactions evaluated with structural equation modeling and fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 7, 768–778. Busch, N. A., Debener, S., Kranczioch, C., Engel, A. K., & Herrmann, C. S. (2004). Size matters: Effects of stimulus size, duration and eccentricity on the visual gamma-band response. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 1810–1820. Cacioppo, J. T., Crites, S. L., Jr., Berntson, G. G., & Coles, M. G. H. (1993). If attitudes affect how stimuli are processed, should they not affect the event-related brain potential? Psychological Science, 4, 108–112. Calvo, M. G., & Lang, P. J. (2005). Parafoveal semantic processing of emotional visual scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 502–519. Codispoti, M., Ferrari, V., & Bradley, M. M. (2006). Repetitive picture processing: Autonomic and cortical correlates. Brain Research, 1068, 213–220. Codispoti, M., Ferrari, V., Junghofer, M., & Schupp, H. T. ¨ (2006). The categorization of natural scenes: Brain attention networks revealed by dense sensor ERPs. NeuroImage, in press. Cuthbert, B. N., Schupp, H. T., Bradley, M. M., Birbaumer, N., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Brain potentials in affective picture processing: Covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biological Psychology, 52, 95–111.

Stimulus size and affective modulation
Detenber, B. H., & Reeves, B. (1996). A bio-informational theory of emotion: Motion and image size effects on viewers. Journal of Communication, 46, 66–84. Fanselow, M. S. (1994). Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 429–438. Freedman, D. J., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T., & Miller, E. K. (2003). A comparison of primate prefrontal and inferior temporal cortices during visual categorization. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5235–5246. Ghashghaei, H. T., & Barbas, H. (2002). Pathways for emotion: Interactions of prefrontal and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience, 115, 1261–1279. Goffaux, V., Gauthier, I., & Rossion, B. (2003). Spatial scale contribution to early visual differences between face and object processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 416–424. Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for offline removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468–484. Harter, M. R., & Aine, C. J. (1984). Brain mechanisms of visual selective attention. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 293–321). New York: Academic Press. Hillyard, S. A., & Anllo-Vento, L. (1998). Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 95, 781–787. Ito, T. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). Tracking the timecourse of social perception: The effects of racial cues on event-related brain potential. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1267–1280. Junghofer, M., Bradley, M. M., Elbert, T. R., & Lang, P. J. (2001). ¨ Fleeting images: A new look at early emotion discrimination. Psychophysiology, 38, 175–178. Junghofer, M., Elbert, T., Tucker, D. M., & Rockstroh, B. (2000). Sta¨ tistical control of artifacts in dense array EEG/MEG studies. Psychophysiology, 37, 523–532. Junghofer, M., & Peyk, P. (2004). Analyzing electrical activity and mag¨ netic fields in the brain. Matlab News & Notes, 2/04, 14–15. Keil, A., Bradley, M. M., Hauk, O., Rockstroh, B., Elbert, T., & Lang, P. J. (2002). Large-scale neural correlates of affective picture processing. Psychophysiology, 39, 641–649. Keil, A., Moratti, S., Sabatinelli, D., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2004). Additive effects of emotional content and spatial selective attention on electrocortical facilitation. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 1187–1197. Kimura, Y., Yoshino, A., Takahashi, Y., & Nomura, S. (2004). Interhemispheric difference in emotional response without awareness. Physiology & Behavior, 82, 727–731. Kok, A. (1997). Event-related potential reflections of mental resources: A review and synthesis. Biological Psychology, 45, 19–56. Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology, 38, 557–577. Kok, A., & De Jong, L. H. (1981). Components of the event-related potential following degraded and undegraded visual stimuli. Biological Psychology, 11, 117–133. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, activation, and action. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons, & M. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting (pp. 97–135). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2001). International affective picture system (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report A-5, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. Loftus, G. R., & Harley, E. M. (2005). Why is it easier to identify someone close than far away? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 43–65. Low, A., Lang, P. J., & Bradley, M. M. (2005). What pops out during ¨ rapid picture presentation? [Abstract]. Psychophysiology, 42, S81. Luck, S. J. (2005). Ten simple rules for designing ERP experiments. In T. C. Handy (Ed.), Event-related potentials: A methods handbook (pp. 17–32). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ´ Mace, M. J. M., Thorpe, S. J., & Fabre-Thorpe, M. (2005). Rapid categorization of achromatic natural scenes: How robust at very low contrasts? European Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 2007–2018.

215
Mangun, G. R. (1995). Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. Psychophysiology, 32, 4–18. Miller, N. (1959). Liberalization of basic S-R concepts. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (vol. 2, pp. 196–292). New York: McGraw-Hill. Muller, M. M., Keil, A., Gruber, T., & Elbert, T. (1999). Processing of ¨ affective pictures modulates right-hemispheric gamma band EEG activity. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 1913–1920. Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resourcelimited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44–64. Perlman, G., Low, A., Keil, A., Calvo, M. G., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, ¨ P. J. (2005). Parafoveal picture presentation: Emotion and attention. [Abstract]. Psychophysiology, 42, S100. Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 99, 11458–11463. Reeves, B., Lang, A., Kim, E. Y., & Tatar, D. (1999). The effects of screen size and message on attention and arousal. Media Psychology, 1, 49–67. Ritter, W., Simson, R., & Vaughan, H. G. (1983). Event-related potential correlates of two stages of information processing in physical and semantic discrimination tasks. Psychophysiology, 20, 168–179. Rolls, E. T. (2000). Neurophysiology and functions of the primate amygdala, and the neural basis of emotion. In J. P. Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala: A functional analysis (pp. 447–478). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Rousselet, G. A. (2005). Fast processing of visual objects: At the interplay between high-level and low-level factors [Abstract]. Psychophysiology, 42, S7. Ruchkin, D. S., & Sutton, S. (1983). Positive slow wave and P300: Association and dissociation. In A. W. K. Gaillard & R. W. Ritter (Eds.), Tutorials in ERP research; endogenous components (pp. 233– 250). Amsterdam: North Holland. Rugg, M. D., Doyle, M. C., & Wells, T. (1995). Word and nonword repetition within and across modality: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 209–227. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools Inc. Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Cacioppo, J. T., Ito, T., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology, 37, 257–261. Schupp, H. T., Junghofer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2003). ¨ Emotional facilitation of sensory processing in the visual cortex. Psychological Science, 14, 7–13. Schupp, H. T., Junghofer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2004). The ¨ selective processing of briefly presented affective pictures: An ERP analysis. Psychophysiology, 41, 441–449. Sigala, N., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Visual categorization shapes feature selectivity in the primate temporal cortex. Nature, 415, 318–320. Stefanacci, L., & Amaral, D. G. (2002). Some observations on cortical inputs to the macaque monkey amygdala: An anterograde tracing study. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 451, 301–323. Teghtsoonian, R., & Frost, R. O. (1982). The effect of viewing distance on fear of snakes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 13, 181–190. Thorpe, S., Fize, D., & Marlot, C. (1996). Speed of processing in the human visual system. Nature, 381, 520–522. VanRullen, R., & Thorpe, S. J. (2001). The time course of visual processing: From early perception to decision-making. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 454–461. Vasey, M. W., & Thayer, J. F. (1987). The continuing problem of false positives in repeated measures ANOVA in psychophysiology: A multivariate solution. Psychophysiology, 24, 479–486.

(Received October 13, 2005; Accepted February 16, 2006)…...

Similar Documents

Free Essay

Task

...Review – Conditionals * Doc 5.5 Cases for Discussion/ Doc 5.6 Crisis Management Scenarios for Discussion | Crisis Management Extra ReadingsICE forum follow up task: Language Review C Discussion | Week 6 Overview Lesson 1 | Homework | Lesson 2 | Homework | * Introduction to Coursework 3 and Portfolio – hand out Student Guidelines for CW3 * Doc.6.1 Evaluation: Introduction to Evaluation – Criteria – Evaluation of SourcesNote: Ss to prepare a seminar for Lesson 2 in Week 8 where they have to introduce their topic, title and sources, and explain why they were chosen (how many, different types, etc.) | 1) Ss do web search on crises, choose topic and write a title for assignment2) Download from ICE & read doc on ‘Research Sources’3) Preview Doc 6.2-A - vocabulary for Titan case study | * Doc 6.2-S Titan Case Study (based on old ML)OR * Doc 6.2-S - Speed Reading Comprehension Questions – ‘Keeping your client relationship afloat’ (based on old ML – see ppt in relevant folder). | Students choose sources and begin note-taking doc for their sourcesSelf –Study:1) Download & read docs on Recording References’ & ‘Using Quotations and Citations’2) Read p.131, ML - Reports | Week 7 Reading Week Week 8 Overview Lesson 1 | Homework | Lesson 2 | Homework | Toyota Case Study – * Doc 8.1 S Task Sheet – Discussion and Listening * Doc 8.2 A – Article 1 - Accelerating towards crisis: a PR view of Toyota’s recall * Doc 8.2 S: Note-taking *......

Words: 461 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Super Size Me

...Extra Credit – Super Size Me In the documentary Super Size Me, there are many messages they are trying to get across. Though most of it is political, there are nutritional messages as well. When Morgan was visiting various doctors in the beginning of the film, many of them were saying the only effect they believed they would see is a rise in triglycerides and blood pressure. None of them seemed too worried about the outcome of this experiment. They all believed that when the whole ordeal was over Morgan would be able to bounce back to his normal physical health. During the experiment, Morgan consumed an average of 5,000 kcal a day. On his second day, he was asked to super size his meal and promptly vomited after consuming his lunch. Even though he couldn’t manage that amount of processed food on that day, only a few days later he found himself craving more only hours after eating. He was becoming addicted to the food. After only five days of the experiment, he had already gained a little over nine pounds. Morgan was finding himself feeling depressed on days that nothing had gone wrong. He was claiming that his depression, lethargy, and headaches were all being relieved when he would eat a meal from McDonalds. On the 21st day he began having heart palpitations. Doctors were advising him to stop the experiment right away and begin detoxing his body from all the harmful substances he has ingested. He refused and continued the rest of the month. As a result from the......

Words: 398 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Analysis of Super Size Me

...In Spurlock’s documentary film Super Size Me, experimentation and interviews confirm Spurlock’s claim because of the interdependent relationship of the two methods. The two social science methods separately do not give sufficient support to validate Spurlock’s claim. If only one of the methods were used, the information gathered would be unsupportive or too broad for the audience to understand. The interviews alone would have only portrayed assumptions and not proof to back up his thesis. If Spurlock solely relied on the experiment, individuals could question the probability of the results happening again or to other people. Interviews are conducted with average Americans and experts to medically explain and prove how fast food meals contribute to the U.S. obesity epidemic. The experiment is used to demonstrate the effectiveness of the fast food industry’s influence to encourage poor nutrition, and to physically show the correlation between fast food and an unhealthy lifestyle. Both methods serve to validate one another because the responses from individuals supply the audience with data and information to better understand Spurlock’s experiment, while his personal experience provides data to support the information the interviewers give. Throughout the length of his experiment, experts are interviewed multiple times, which is crucial to reveal and explain the negative effects of fast food to the human body. As Spurlock prepares himself mentally, he sets appointments and......

Words: 1034 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

Population Size

...Population Size | 5.18 million | Population Growth | 0.817% | Age distribution | 0-14 years: 13.8% 15-64 years: 77%65 years and over: 9.2% | Degree of Urbanization | 0.9% annual rate of change | GDP/ GDP growth | 84,237 / 1.6% | Contribution of GDP growth by manufacturing, service and agricultural sector | Manufacturing: -0.2%Service: 1.3%Agricultural sector: - | Purchasing power/PPP | $314.5 billion [US dollars] | Inflation | 4.6% | Economic System | Free market economy | Major export and import products | Exports: machinery and equipment, consumer goods, pharmaceuticals and other chemicals, mineral fuelsImports: machinery and equipment, mineral fuels, chemicals, food and consumer goods | Major export and import tradition partners | Exports: Malaysia 11.9%, Hong Kong 11.7%, China 10.4%, Indonesia 9.4%, US 6.5%, Japan 4.7%, South Korea 4.1%Import: Malaysia 11.7%, US 11.5%, China 10.8%, Japan 7.9%, South Korea 5.8%, Indonesia 5.4% | Balance of Payment | 6,264 million dollars | * Infrastructure development ASEAN Framework Agreement on Multimodal Transport (AFAMT) is a critical support of transportation sector to the logistical and service in regional and international trade, ASEAN had worked to facilitate the door-to-door delivery of goods using various modes of transport under a single transport document with the AFAMT. * Per capita consumption level of your product if applicable * Country development level......

Words: 832 - Pages: 4

Free Essay

Size Zero Fad

...What's the common and sexy link between Posh Spice and Kareena Kapoor? Their size zero figures! It's been the talk of the international fashion scene lately and especially back home after Kareena dropped oodles of weight for her new look in the much maligned film Tashan. However, as BT discovered, there exists no such thing as a size zero, really. So what are Posh and Bebo talking about? Designer Manish Malhotra said size zero is so not in, not even in Bollywood, that factory of dreams. "You've got to have a bustline and a good butt to complement your waistline. But in getting a slim waist, most women's bodies go flat, and that's a turn off for most men," said Manish "I have never designed any clothes for size zero. Abroad, many women flaunt a slim waist and get a boob and butt job done to get that accentuated look. But in India, a woman tries to get that look naturally and ends up going all flat. Size zero's so not done, girls. Get back to your curves." Designer Nikasha Tawade, on the other hand, said size zero does exist, but it's American sizing. "To me, this size means the girl has either disappeared into thin air or is just pre-pubescent. And am glad, I've never had the privilege to meet anyone of that size." Meanwhile, are Posh and Bebo really looked upon for their waif-thin looks? Posh is nicknamed 'Skeletal Spice' abroad because of her twig-like frame. In fact, she has turned down women like herself from modelling her denim range. Her belief is that......

Words: 637 - Pages: 3

Free Essay

Primate Size and Sexual Dimorphism

...Primate Size and Sexual Dimorphism Sexual Dimorphism in Primate Evolution by J. Michael Plavcan and Correlates of Sexual Dimorphism in Primates: Ecological and Size Variables by Walter Leutenegger and James Cheverud are very similar articles for their respective journals, but do come to a major disagreement within their articles. The agreements come when discussing which primates are monogamous and polygynous, why this is the case, and why sexual dimorphism will continue at a forever-growing rate through time. Plavcan disagrees with Leutnegger and Cheverud on one key fundamental issue, saying that the study of sexual dimorphism as it relates to the size of primates, their mating system, and how this came to be. Similarities Plavcan, Leutnegger, and Cheverud reached the same conclusion when correlating the size of the primate and the likelihood of that primate being monogamous or polygynous. Quite simply, the bigger the primate, the more likely they are to be polygynous. The three cite a few reasons for this. The first reason is because of sexual selection. Polygynous males often fight for the affection of female primates and the bigger the primate, the more likely they are to succeed. This is why hominoids such as apes are polygynous, whereas smaller primates like Gibbons stay with one mate throughout their lives. The three scientists also do not consider it a coincidence that males are much larger and more advanced in their evolution of dimorphism. Consider the......

Words: 676 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Super-Size Me Essay

...Watching the moving “Super-Size Me” never fails to both intrigue and completely disgust me all at the same time. “Fast food” to me is exactly what it says it is: food that is processed, cooked, served, and eaten fast. I have never really been one to go “anti-fast-food” but after seeing “Super-Size Me” for the first time, it took me a few weeks to be able to pick up a McDonalds chicken nugget. However, a few big questions have arrived from the movie (after seeing it a few times I still get grossed out at the thought). A big question that really was brought up for me was the tendencies that people are holding fast food companies responsible for their obesity problems. I think that is just absolutely ridiculous. They chose to eat there, and I’m sure they know the risks of eating this food. It’s pretty much common knowledge that fast food is VERY unhealthy. The people who sued McDonalds probably were not exercising at all or eating anything healthy at home. If they were really worried about their health, they would ask about the nutrition facts and try to watch their weight and what they eat. If somebody gets fat, it’s nothing but their own fault. This idea presents a very important aspect of American society as a whole: that a lack of education, selfishness, and greed seem to drive American society in a whole lot of ways. This includes why obesity is so dominant in American culture. We want things fast, already prepared, and without any effort on our part, so of course fast......

Words: 349 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Super Size Me

...Super Size Me Assignment To begin with, I have seen this movie Super Size Me before in high school. This is my second time watching it and I am still shocked by what is said in this movie. With that being said, I do eat fast food quite often. I eat it about 2-3 times a week. I do not live at home so I no longer get home cooked meals from my mom. I try to cook by myself sometimes but there are only so many things I know how to make and is sometimes more convenient for me just to drive down the street and get Del Taco or Weiner Schnitzel’s. Also with work and school I try my best to bring my own lunch or eat at a restaurant rather than going to a fast food joint. This film relates to the NF15 course in many ways, one of them being that when the narrator interviewed random people on the streets, nobody knew what a calorie was. This shows that people hear the word calories and see it on every food label and everywhere they look that there’s food, but still do not know what it is. This goes to show that people do not generally care about calories and how much they take in. Most people who eat fast food also do not know how many calories they are taking in from the food that they ordered. The foods ordered from fast food restaurants do not have food labels and only some will give you a nutrition facts sheet if asked for one. The film also showed that an order of small fries had 200 calories while the large order had 600 calories. That is 3 times the amount of......

Words: 1584 - Pages: 7

Premium Essay

Size Should Not Matter

...Size Should Not Matter: Being Over a Size Ten Does Not Make One Ugly Is brand image more important than satisfying the most customers possible? In the mind of Mike Jeffries, CEO of Abercrombie & Fitch (A&F), yes brand image is more important than satisfying the most customers possible. According to Jeffries, A&F has a very selective target market, which does not include anyone who is overweight or in Jeffries’s mind who is not “good-looking”. In my opinion, even though the company A&F believes it has a certain brand image to maintain, there is no reason why A&F should discriminate against the amount of people that they do. By considering Aristotelian virtue ethics, Mill’s utilitarianism, and Carr’s idea on business bluffing, it is easy to see why the tactics of A&F are unmoral, or in the case of Carr, unjustified. On the other side, Kantian deontology shows that the tactics of A&F can be considered moral, however Kant’s reasons are not stronger than the reasons of Aristotle, Mill, and Carr. Before getting into the analyses from the perspectives from the previously mentioned people, it would be ideal to give some background information on the unethical values of A&F. It was not until 1996 when Jeffries became fully in charge of A&F when the company started to become an unethical store. The main reason the company has become so unethical, was because of the new brand image Jeffries decided to create for the company. Instead of having a brand image that could be marketed......

Words: 3638 - Pages: 15

Premium Essay

Effects of Magnet Size

...The Effect of Magnet Size in Producing Electricity Ma. Cariza P. Francisco Linette Verdillo Joan Grace Gumasing Mia Madellaine Mendoza Emmanuel Rivera In partial fulfillment of the requirements in Physics Liceo de Buenavista Mrs. Nerisa Gonzales March 7, 2011 Table of Contents ABSTRACT …………………………………………………………………..i ACKNOWLEDGEMENT…………………………………………………….ii I. INTRODUCTION ……………………………………………………………1 * Background of the Study……………………………………………...1 * Statement of the problem……………………………………………..1 * Hypothesis……………………………………………………………..1 * Research Paradigm……………………………………………………2 * Significance of the study………………………………………….…..3 * Scope and Delimitation………………………………………………..3 II. REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE…………………………………….4 * Definition of Term……………………………………………….…....7 III. METHODOLOGY……………………………………………………….…...8 IV. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION …………………………………………….12 V. CONCLUSION/ RECOMMENDATION…………………………………...13 BIBLIOGRAPHY……………………………………………………………iii ABSTRACT This study aims to evaluate the potential of magnet size in producing electricity. This magnet producing electricity was conducted to make a substitute light in case of urgent situation as a replacement for using flashlight that needs batteries. The researchers assembled the electric generator by inserting magnet in hollow ended-box. Poke a hole in center of the box using a nail. At this point you should let four magnets clamps themselves around the nail. Coil......

Words: 3558 - Pages: 15

Premium Essay

Nike Common Size Statement

...Nike Inc. Common Size Consolidated Income Statement 2014 Revenues 100.00 Cost of Sales -55.23 Gross Profit 44.77% Demand Creation Expense -10.90 Operating overhead expense -20.63 Selling & Administrative Expense -31.53% Restructing charges Goodwill impairment Intangible & other asset impairment Operating income 13.24% Interest income 0.02 Interest expense -0.14 Interest (expense) net -0.12% Other income (expense) net -0.37 Income before income taxes 12.75% Income tax expense -3.06 Net income from discontinuing operations 9.69% Net income from discontinued operations Net Income 9.69% Nike Common Size Statement of Financial Position, Liabilities, & Stockholder Equity 2014 Current portion of long term debt 0.04 Notes payable 0.90 Accounts payable 0.38 Compensation & Benefits, excluding taxes 4.21 Endorsement compensation 1.76 Dividends payable 1.12 Taxes other than income taxes 1.10 Advertising & Marketing 0.72 Import & Logistics costs 0.68 Fair value of derivatives 0.46 Other 3.35 Accrued Liabilities 13.40% Income taxes payable 2.32 Liabilities of discontinued operations Current liabilities 27.04% Long term debt, excluding current portion 6.45 Deferred income taxes and other liabilities 8.30 Noncurrent liabilities 14.75% Total liabilities 41.79% Redeemable preferred......

Words: 391 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Tasks

...assess users satisfaction levels towards their service so that they can use the feedback to make positive adjustments to their products and services. Iravo et. al., (2013) state that dissatisfied customers will be disloyal to the organization and will talk about their bad experience to other customers. In this study CRM is viewed as a customer strategy for retaining customer loyalty and improving hotels performance. 2.2 Strategic planning and hotel performance The history of strategic planning dates back to long-range planning (Cappelli, 2005). Strategic planning was therefore a proactive alternative to long-range planning which was found to be obsolete because it was not increasing firm’s true value. Strategic Planning is a core task of senior management which involves fourteen (14) processes (Armstrong, 2010). These processes are designing objectives, planning strategy, establishing goals, developing company philosophy, policies, procedures, organization structures, establishing personnel and facilities, capital, establishing standards, programs and operational plans and 86 International Journal of Arts and Commerce Vol. 2 No. 11 December, 2013 institutionalization, evaluation and control. Pearce and Robinson (2008) view Strategic Planning as an organizational process that is vision driven and that aims at developing the future value of an organization. Dan (2009) states that Strategic Planning process involves the implementation of strategy in an...

Words: 11117 - Pages: 45

Premium Essay

Hydrometer Particle Size Analysis

...Particle Size Analysis - Hydrometer CEE340-340L Engineering Geology and Lab Prepared for: Instructor: Kyungan (Karen) Min, Ph.D, P.E. Teaching Assistant: Casey Janisch Prepared by: Abdelrahman Mohsen Submitted On: 10/28/2014 Introduction: The particle size analysis is widely used in classification of soils. The data obtained from particle size distribution curves is used in the design of filters for dams and to determine the suitability of soil for road construction and air fields. Hydrometer test is used to determine what type of clay is predominant in a given soil to be later used by Civil Engineers in construction. Objective: To determine the particle-size distribution of a given soil sample for the fraction that is finer than No. 200 sieve size (0.075 mm). The lower limit of the particle-size determined by this procedure is about 0.001 mm. In hydrometer analysis, a soil specimen is dispersed in water. In a dispersed state in the water, the soil particles will settle at different velocities over time. The hydrometer will measure the specific gravity of the soil-water suspension. Hydrometer readings will be taken at specific time intervals to measure the percentage of soil still in suspension at time t. From this data the percentage of soil by weight finer and the diameters (D) of the soil particles at their respective time readings can be calculated. A graph of the diameter (D) vs. percent finer can be plotted to develop a particle-size......

Words: 1166 - Pages: 5

Free Essay

Full Size or Compact

...Full Size or Compact The love people have for horses have been around for centuries. There are many breeds of horses; the most popular is the Quarter Horse. Over the years a smaller breed of the Quarter Horse has emerged, the Quarter Pony. There is certain criteria a Quarter Horse must have, the Quarter Pony must meet certain criteria, and since the Quarter Pony comes from the Quarter Horse breed the two are alike and different. Even though the Quarter Pony is a small replica of the Quarter Horse, the Quarter Horse is one of the most recognized horse breeds in America. The Quarter Horse has certain criteria it must have to be considered for registration with the American Quarter Horse Association (AQHA). The following is some of the AQHA regulations that are required (AQHA, 2011). The horse must be 14.2 Hands High (HH) or taller, but not over 15.3 HH, must be from a bloodline of registered Quarter Horses (mother and father must be registered with the AQHA), the horse must be one of the 17 basic Quarter Horse coat colors, and must be tall, muscular, lean, and fast. The quarter Pony has certain criteria it must meet to register with the International Quarter Pony Association (IQPA). The following is some of the IQPA regulations that are required (IQPA, 2011). The horse is required to be under 14.2 HH and over 11.2 HH, must come from one parent that is registered with the AQHA (the mother or father must be registered with the AQHA), the horse does not have to be one...

Words: 512 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Class Size and Student Achievement

...Class size is an issue that appears to be straightforward when looking at it from a teacher’s point of view; however, there is a great deal of debate over the effectiveness. The main issue, as I see it, is this: What matters more, the class size or the teacher? Before researching articles concerning class size, I believe smaller classes do indeed result in better test scores. At the same time though I consider myself an effective teacher, so with smaller class sizes I feel that I can be even more effective in getting my point across to the students. Through the articles that I am going to research, I hope to find opinions that support my thoughts, find differing opinions from my own, and I hope to be able to see a “trend” that I can predict based on economic, social, and political issues. So far in my educational career I have experienced many different class sizes, from thirty in one class to nine in a class. I have taught classes ranging from Pre-Algebra with enrichment students to Algebraic Thinking where I am teaching “disadvantaged” and “at-risk” students. This has allowed me to not only see a wide range of different types and sizes of classes, but it has also allowed me time to form my opinion. It is my belief that an effective teacher is what matters the most in a classroom, but class does certainly helps. As McKenzie writes, “It probably is best to have both small classes and effective teachers,” but he agrees that he would “rather have a really good......

Words: 1302 - Pages: 6